Tuesday 29 July 2014

Chronic Pain: A Modern Problem

Today's very long post from painresearchforum.org (see link below) is worth the space it takes up because it shows the results of a symposium on biological and psychological factors that influence chronic pain. Many neuropathy patients are experts on living with chronic pain and can also tell you a great deal about which factors influence it in their daily lives. Because chronic pain is a hot topic at the moment (mainly because of the health costs required to sustain it), this sort of conference and in-depth discussion means that the authorities are waking up to the fact that people living with chronic pain are not serial fakers or hypochondriacs and that many other factors come into play that affect the seriousness of the condition. Definitely worth a read.

Pain and Depression: A Comorbidity Conundrum
News from the 2014 NIH Pain Consortium Symposium on biological and psychological factors that contribute to chronic pain
by Pat McCaffrey on 9 Jul 2014


Interest in pain is on the rise at the US National Institutes of Health (NIH), said Nora Volkow, chief of the National Institute on Drug Abuse, welcoming participants to the 9th Annual NIH Pain Consortium Symposium, held May 28-29, 2014, in Bethesda, US. Volkow noted a significant increase in research funding at the NIH for chronic pain conditions, which amounted to $400 million in 2014, up from $279 million in 2008. That is still just about 1 percent of the entire NIH pie, but in an era of shrinking budgets overall, that is no small progress.

The increase follows the 2010 Affordable Care Act (aka Obamacare), which mandated a hard look at the state of pain education, care, and research. That legislation led to the 2011 report from the Institutes of Medicine on the public health impact of pain (see PRF related story and commentary).

For those who want to know where that money and other federal funds are going, the NIH recently announced the launch of a database of all the federal grants related to pain (see press release and news coverage). The Interagency Pain Research Portfolio is a publicly accessible, searchable resource that for the first time collects information on all federal research, including efforts by the NIH, the Centers for Disease Control, the Department of Defense, the Food and Drug Administration, and others in one place.The database reflects the Pain Consortium’s mission to enhance pain research and promote collaboration across government agencies.

Volkow also announced that on September 29-30, 2014, the NIH Office of Disease Prevention will sponsor a workshop on the role of opioids in treating chronic pain. Prescription opioid misuse, addiction, and overdose deaths have grabbed the attention of physicians, patients, the media, and policy makers, but data that would support the proper use of these drugs for chronic pain are severely limited. The workshop will tackle the scientific issues around opioid use for chronic pain. Registration will open in June. For more information, see 2014 NIH Pathways to Prevention workshop on The Role of Opioids in the Treatment of Chronic Pain.

The theme of this year’s symposium was biological and psychological factors that contribute to chronic pain. Over one and a half days, speakers discussed comorbid factors including depression, sleep disturbances, and inflammation. Summaries of selected talks on pain and depression are presented here; Part 2 deals with pain and sleep. In addition, an archived webcast of the entire meeting is available on the NIH website (view Day 1 and Day 2). The complete meeting agenda is here.

Pain and depression—what is the relationship?

Pain can certainly be depressing, and some data suggest that depression increases the risk of pain. But in his overview talk on the topic, Dan Clauw, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, US, said that getting to a precise understanding of how often the two conditions co-occur is actually not easy. One problem is that the clinical instruments designed to identify depression are often “contaminated” by measures, such as sleep disturbances and headaches, that are also present in chronic pain conditions. Clauw said that going forward, studies seeking to measure comorbidity will need to isolate the cognitive and affective aspects of depression from somatic symptoms that are shared with chronic pain.

In people who do have pain and depression, which comes first? Nearly all studies suggest that chronic pain increases the risk of depression, but a fair number of data also suggests the opposite: that people with a history of depression are at a higher risk for chronic pain. Clauw noted that the magnitude of the increased risk for pain is modest (less than a twofold increase), and when measured in similar studies, other factors seem more important: Early life stressors, other psychiatric conditions, prior pain, prior somatic symptoms, and poor sleep are all stronger predictors of subsequent chronic pain. “Depression, although a risk, is not one of the stronger risks for development of subsequent pain,” Clauw said. Nonetheless, depression could be an important absolute risk factor for pain because it is so common.

In considering future directions, Clauw raised what he called “the 800-pound gorilla in the room”—what central nervous system factor or factors should researchers focus on when thinking about chronic pain? “It’s not depression, or catastrophizing. It’s something I don’t have a name for, but I see it over and over,” he said. The “something” manifests in a quartet of symptoms that cut across many diseases and include widespread pain, fatigue, memory and mood problems (including depression), and sleep disturbances.

Clauw, a rheumatologist, said he saw this in people who had consumed contaminated tryptophan in the 1980s. They first developed eosinophilia myalgia syndrome, but when the acute inflammatory phase resolved, these individuals looked like they had a severe form of fibromyalgia. He also saw an epidemic of this in veterans of the first Gulf War, and in a very high proportion of veterans from the Iraq and Afghanistan conflicts who suffer from the “polytrauma triad” of centralized pain, post-traumatic stress disorder, and mild traumatic brain injury.

This disorder goes by several names: fibromyalgia; somatization, or somatoform disorder (which involves pain in multiple areas early in life); and central sensitization, or regional idiopathic pain. This same symptom complex is very common in diseases such as interstitial cystitis, tension headache, irritable bowel, temporomandibular joint disease, vulvodynia, and dry eye. Clauw said that in his tertiary clinic, he sees patients who have had untreated disease for 30 years. These are people who have had pain in many regions over their lives and have been seen by many specialists who have a tendency to focus on where the pain is at that point in the patients' lives rather than on a lifelong history of pain in multiple areas of the body. “These are the people that all subspecialists dislike: They are a huge clinical problem,” Clauw said. “In aggregate, this may be one of the most common clinical diseases known to man, and may be the most disabling.”

This group of symptoms seems to represent some type of centralized pain state. Clauw believes that different chronic pain conditions differ in the proportion of individuals who “centralize” their pain. On the low end of a continuum are primarily nociceptive conditions such as osteoarthritis, where only 15-20 percent of people will clearly exhibit centralization, to the other end of the continuum, where nearly all patients exhibit this (e.g., fibromyalgia). It’s important to tease this out in each patient, he said, because different drugs are effective for each type of pain.

Currently, the best way to approximate this centralized state, Clauw thinks, is the concept of “FM-ness.” The current FM diagnostic criteria involve assessment of pain location and severity, as well as fatigue, depression, and other symptoms, generating an aggregate score that can range from 0 to 31 (Wolfe et al., 2011). The cut-off score for probable fibromyalgia is 13, but Clauw said the scale can also be viewed as a continuous measure of the centralization of pain, with low scores reflecting mainly nociceptive conditions, and higher scores indicating a larger degree of central nervous system involvement.

To support this idea, Clauw showed data from his ongoing study looking at the relationship between FM score and postoperative outcomes of osteoarthritis patients after knee or hip replacement surgery (Brummett et al., 2013). In the study, the researchers assessed patients before surgery for a battery of known risk factors for postoperative pain, but added a new factor—the FM score based on the 2011 revised diagnostic criteria (Wolfe et al., 2011). Only 9 percent of the 519 subjects studied met FM criteria—most subjects had scores between 5 and 10, and when Clauw analyzed outcomes for the patients with scores below 13, he found that the FM score predicted everything bad: post-operative pain, anxiety, and depression. The FM score highly predicted post-operative opioid use—for each one-point increase, patients needed an additional 9 mg opioids per day, and many still had ongoing pain. In terms of predicting six-month pain and global outcomes, the FM score subsumes all other variables they measured, Clauw said.

The FM score is not an ideal measure, Clauw said. “We hate this measure and are trying to move away from this. We think there are relative weightings of the items on this questionnaire or other questions that could be added that would improve the measure, and that it will be particularly important to validate this against biological measures such as brain imaging. He thinks that they are looking at some kind of central nervous system condition that starts in childhood, persists into adulthood, and "severely challenges the historical notion that most chronic pain is due to a problem in the region of the body where the individual hurts."

Mechanistic clues from animal work

A. Courtney DeVries, Ohio State University, Columbus, US, presented new animal work exploring the physiological mechanisms by which stress and social factors influence the risk of pain and depression after nerve injury. Previously, she had shown that stress (induced by two hours of daily restraint for two weeks) exacerbated allodynia and depressive-like behavior in the forced swim test after spared nerve injury in male mice. Stress alone did not induce allodynia or depressive-like behavior, but it made both worse via a mechanism involving induction of IL1beta expression in the brain (Norman et al., 2010). DeVries noted that while all of this work was done in male mice, she and her colleagues have since looked at females and do not see sex differences.

In more recent work, DeVries looked at the effects of social interaction on pain and depression in the same model. She found a reduction in allodynia and depressive behaviors in animals housed in pairs before injury compared to animals reared alone. In addition, if animals were paired after injury, she could see a significant decrease in allodynia.

Looking at the combined effects of pairing and stress on pain, DeVries showed that pairing in stressed animals improved allodynia, but did not completely eliminate it. Animals reared in isolation and exposed to stress fared worst of all. She has not yet looked at effect of pairing on the depressive behaviors, but predicts that those behaviors will be more amenable than allodynia to improvement with social interaction.

How might pairing improve pain? DeVries thinks that, like stress, the effect of pairing may be mediated by changes in IL1beta, because giving IL1beta to paired animals increased allodynia to the level seen in socially isolated mice. In contrast, treating socially isolated mice with IL1 receptor antagonist reduced allodynia to levels comparable to paired mice. The results may apply to other forms of pain—DeVries said she can demonstrate the effect of social interaction and stress on ischemia-induced allodynia, too.

For treatment, accentuate the positive

In her talk, Mary Davis, Arizona State University, Tempe, US, turned around the question of pain and depression. She asked why the pain field focuses so much on negative mood and emotions when data from other fields suggest that the presence of a positive affect predicts good outcomes better than negative affect predicts bad outcomes (e.g., see Southwick and Charney, 2012). Davis, who works on pain and mood regulation in women with chronic pain, asked whether, rather than focus mainly on diminishing negative moods, clinicians should pay more attention to actively promoting positive mood and emotions to build resilience in the face of chronic pain?

That idea is supported by her recent data (Davis et al., 2014) showing that in depressed women with pain, inducing a positive mood improved the response to stress in terms of pain. In the study, Davis looked at 110 women with fibromyalgia and/or osteoarthritis, of whom 29 percent were depressed. The subjects were subjected to a stress-inducing interview and then randomly shown either a humorous film clip or a neutral film clip. Afterwards, they were queried about their mood and pain.

The stress interview caused both depressed and non-depressed subjects to become more despondent and report more pain. After viewing the film clip, the non-depressed subjects showed a spontaneous rebound to better mood, regardless of whether they saw a positive or neutral clip. However, the depressed women rebounded only if they viewed the funny clip. The changes in mood tracked with clinical pain: The depressed subjects reported that their pain was reduced only in the positive mood induction group. The results suggest that interventions that serve to boost positive mood post-stress in depressed chronic pain patients could help lessen pain.

In a test of the clinical efficacy of mood improvement, Davis and colleagues had previously showed that mindfulness-based therapy, which enhances positive affect, pain coping, and resilience, was more effective than cognitive-behavioral therapy (which focuses on restructuring negative thoughts) or education at reducing joint tenderness in depressed people with rheumatoid arthritis (Zautra et al., 2008). When looking at daily diaries in patients from that study, Davis detected an increase in daily resilience with mindfulness training—on high-pain days, the patients showed less catastrophizing and less emotional reactivity to stress. This was not true for CBT or education.

More recently, the group demonstrated the possibility of delivering mindfulness-based therapies online aimed at boosting positive affect and social engagement. In a randomized trial, Davis and her group compared the effects of mindfulness training to general health tips, delivered online to 79 fibromyalgia patients. The groups showed no changes in pain after completing the short (four-hour total) intervention, but everyone improved on negative affect. Mindfulness was better than health tips at boosting positive affect, enjoyment of family, and pain coping efficacy (Davis and Zautra, 2013). Although the effects were small, the results are promising and highlight the potential of Internet-based delivery of low-cost and widely accessible psychological interventions for improving pain coping skills.

From pain and depression, the NIH Pain Consortium Symposium agenda moved to pain and sleep disorders. Michael Vitiello, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, US, described results of a randomized clinical trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for comorbid insomnia and pain. Vitiello, a sleep researcher, said his move to study pain has been inspired by accumulating evidence that one way to improve chronic pain may be to treat the comorbid sleep disturbances that frequently come along with it.

The relationship between sleep and pain, like the pain-depression relationship, is complex. Previously, most assumed that pain caused sleep disturbance, but new thinking holds that insomnia might actually contribute to pain. As far back as 1924, researchers knew that sleeplessness enhanced pain sensitivity, and a growing body of data suggests that experimental disruptions of sleep can lead to increased pain (Roehrs, 2009). Now, Vitiello and colleagues are producing data suggesting that improving sleep can improve pain.

For people with insomnia, cognitive-behavioral therapy can help. Vitiello said that CBT is “robust for sleep and can improve sleep even in the presence of ongoing pain.” But results of the treatment on pain have been mixed. In a previous study of CBT for insomnia, Vitiello found that people with osteoarthritis experienced improvements in both insomnia and pain (Vitiello et al., 2009). That result led him to devise a study to compare the effect of CBT for pain with a combination of CBT for pain plus insomnia. The study involved 367 older adults, mostly women, who received weekly 90-minute training sessions for six weeks.

The results of that study showed that the intervention reduced insomnia but not pain after nine months. At 18 months, there was no effect on either endpoint, suggesting that the combined therapy not only did not improve pain, but might have actually diluted the effectiveness for insomnia (McCurry et al., 2014).

But a post-hoc analysis reveals more reason for hope. When the researchers broke the subjects into groups based on severity of pain and insomnia, they found that the treatment actually worked for the most severe cases, with significant decreases in pain achieved at 18 months in the 98 most affected subjects.

Next, in additional analysis, the results of which have just been published (Vitiello et al., 2014), the researchers looked at people whose sleep improved at two months (regardless of which study arm they were assigned to) versus non-improvers and found that short-term sleep improvers showed continued improvement in sleep as well as decreases in pain severity and arthritis symptoms at 18 months. From this, Vitiello concluded that successful treatment of sleep disturbances in the short term may reduce pain in the long term. He is now proposing to conduct a larger randomized trial in severely affected people, with delivery of therapy by phone.

Monika Haack, Harvard Medical School, Boston, US, reported on her studies of the effects of sleep deprivation and insomnia on inflammation, pain sensitivity, and pain modulation. Haack has found that people with primary insomnia (sleeplessness with no apparent medical cause) report pain on twice as many days as controls, and report increased pain sensitivity. Many studies on evoked pain show sleep deprivation decreases pain thresholds, and Haack showed the same in the insomnia subjects, along with a reduced ability to modulate pain (Haack et al., 2012).

To study sleep deprivation in the lab, Haack brings subjects into a clinical research center, where for 14 days she can rigorously control sleep times, food and drink, and physical and social activity. In healthy volunteers, she has demonstrated that experimental sleep deprivation causes an increase in spontaneous reporting of pain, and decreases in conditioned pain modulation.

To look for physiological changes that accompany sleep deficits, Haack focused on changes in inflammatory markers in people subjected to sleep restriction and recovery in a pattern that mimicked the common experience of sleep restriction on work or school days and catch-up sleep over the weekend. When subjects were put through repeated cycles of restriction and recovery, she saw increases in the cytokine interleukin 6 (IL6) that became progressively worse with each cycle. Even though subjects felt refreshed after the recovery sleep phase, IL6 levels did not return to normal. This suggests that the subjective feeling of recovery from sleep deprivation may not be reflected by resetting of biological systems, Haack said. In addition, an increase in spontaneous pain with sleep deprivation correlated with IL6 levels and with an increase in urine prostaglandins. Together, the results suggest that inflammation induced by sleep deprivation may contribute to the excess pain reported by sleep-deprived people.

A clean sweep with sleep

What is sleep for, anyway? That question has long puzzled neurobiologists. In her talk, Maiken Nedergaard, University of Rochester, US, outlined recent findings pointing to sleep as a crucial interlude that allows the brain to clean house metabolically by flushing out toxic metabolites through the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Nedergaard recently discovered that the flushing process occurs mainly during sleep: In her talk, she presented some new data about how pain can have a significant impact on the brain’s hygiene.

The brain makes up just a fraction of total body weight, but it uses almost 25 percent of the body’s glucose, which means it must produce a lot of waste. The brain lacks a lymphatic system like the one that drains interstitial fluid, and waste, from other tissues, so just how it keeps clean has been unclear. It has been known for some time that CSF flows through the brain in channels alongside the vasculature, its movement driven by the pulsing of the large arteries (Rennels et al., 1985). Nedergaard developed a method to directly visualize that flow in living animals using florescent tracers and in-vivo two-photon video microscopy. In mice, the tracer can be seen moving first along the vasculature and then into the brain tissue spaces before being cleared with the exiting CSF, which drains alongside large veins. Astrocytes facilitate the exchange of substances between CSF and brain interstitial fluid, so Nedergaard coined the term glymphatic system (a combination of glia and lymphatic) to describe this brain-cleaning mechanism.

In a high-profile study published last year, Nedergaard and colleagues showed that glymphatic flow is most active during sleep. When they compared tracer flow in mice anesthetized with ketamine, or naturally asleep, or just waking up, they saw an active transfer of tracer in anesthetized or sleeping mice, but not in awake mice (Xie et al., 2013).

Nedergaard wanted to know how pain might affect glymphatic function. She did a spinal nerve ligation in mice and looked at tracer distribution after three days. In normal, awake mice, little or no tracer enters the brain. After spinal nerve ligation, when the mice were in pain, there was even less tracer than in normal awake mice. In anesthetized, injured mice, there was a slight increase in tracer in CSF, but Nedergaard said she was not yet sure if this was real because the results with ketamine are always more variable.

Because glymphatic function runs opposite to excitation and alertness, Nedergaard speculated that pain might reflect a state of high alert, and thus low waste clearance. The study, while preliminary, raises several interesting questions: Do people in pain need more sleep? Does glymphatic flow contribute to the clearance of inflammatory cytokines, and if so, could increasing clearance reduce pain? And finally, could imaging of clearance provide a novel diagnostic measure of pain?

Crowd sourcing for better treatments

The symposium was also a chance to unveil a new resource for pain treatment and research. Sean Mackey, Stanford University, Palo Alto, US, introduced a new registry designed for gathering clinical data, now in use at his Stanford pain clinic. The goal, he said, is to capture outcome and other data on patients who visit the pain clinic, and to then take advantage of that mass of data to improve patient care and research.

To do that, Mackey and colleagues at Stanford collaborated with the NIH to develop the Stanford-NIH Health Electronic Registry of Outcomes (HERO), an open-source, open-standard, highly flexible and free health and treatment registry. The system allows the collection of, and easy access to, outcome and other data on large numbers of pain patients. Having the data on hand will enable physicians to make better decisions for individual patients based on the patient’s own history and the accumulated data of all patients. The data will also allow comparative effectiveness research as well as “pragmatic” or “practice-based evidence” trials, which analyze outcomes of different treatments in clinical populations (see PRF related news story).

Mackey said he has been collecting these kinds of data at the Stanford pain management center for the last 15 years, starting with a pen and paper, and then using software programs, but he needed more. HERO incorporates easy data import from electronic medical records and collection of patient outcomes based on the NIH Patient Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS).

In HERO, patients enter data using electronic questionnaires. Because of the electronic format, the questionnaires are adaptive, which means that, depending on the answers to initial questions, subsequent questions are tailored mid-test. Mackey said this allows for questionnaires that cover a wide range of situations with many fewer questions, so patients take less time and provide more useful information than older pen-and-paper instruments. PROMIS data are also normative—scores are compared to population norms, allowing pooling and data comparison. Patients enter data at home or in the office on phones and other mobile devices.

The system was rolled out in August 2012, Mackey said, and currently has roughly 10,000 data points on 3,500 patients. The adoption has occurred with minimal complaints from staff or patients. Mackey said the system “has changed the culture of how we practice pain management. Providers use this all the time for teaching and for providing care.”

Mackey showed one example of a woman with chronic regional pain syndrome (CRPS). She had scored above normal on measures of depression and anxiety, and was in the lowest 10 percent of the population on measures of physical function. After initial pharmacological treatment, questionnaire data revealed evidence of improvement in mood and less pain interference, but no change in her physical function. So Mackey gave her a health educator, and by the next visit the patient did show an improvement in physical function. “We can watch what happens over time on multiple measures and adjust treatment accordingly,” Mackey explained. “For some patients, there seems to be some kind of barrier to improvement that shows up early. If they can get over that, they continue to improve,” he said. “We can use this system to ask what that barrier is and how to get people over it. “

Another useful output of the system is the generation of population-based information on the clinical population of pain patients. Comparing the Stanford clinic population to US norms, Mackey showed that his patients skew to the top percentiles for measures of pain, anxiety, fatigue, pain interference, depression, and physical function. But some patients are different—they appear happy and calm, and the system allows Mackey and colleagues to easily identity these interesting outliers with severe pain and dysfunction, but no anxiety, depression, and anger. “What can we learn from them?” he asked.

HERO started as a pain registry, but has morphed into a general health registry, and the group is now working on other versions including Headache HERO, Orthopedic HERO, and GI HERO. Built on industry-standard, non-proprietary tools, the system can accommodate any kind of survey. Mackey has plans for future enhancement and is looking for partners who want to add in features.

Junior investigators highlighted

Each year at the symposium, a junior investigator is recognized with the Mitchell Max Award, given for the best poster presentation. The award honors the late Max, a visionary pain researcher at the NIH and the University of Pittsburgh, US. The 2014 prize went to Fadel Zeidan, a postdoctoral fellow at Wake Forest School of Medicine, North Carolina, US, for work investigating the analgesic mechanisms of mindfulness meditation. Zeidan compared the effect of mindfulness meditation, sham mindfulness meditation, a placebo-conditioning regimen, or a neutral intervention on thermal pain sensitivity and brain activity (measured by arterial spin labeling MRI) in a total of 75 healthy volunteers (17-20 per intervention). In the study, the mindfulness intervention was most effective, reducing pain intensity by 26 percent and unpleasantness by 44 percent. Placebo and sham meditation also reduced pain intensity and unpleasantness significantly (by 19 and 24 percent), but to a lesser extent. In the imaging arm of the study, meditation was found to significantly deactivate the thalamus and the periaqueductal grey matter more than placebo, sham meditation, or control conditions. These results, together with Zeidan’s previous work, suggest that mindfulness meditation probably attenuates pain through multiple mechanisms, while placebo and sham meditation likely engage the expected descending control processes. In his presentation, Zeidan stated that mindfulness-based studies employing robust comparison conditions may better help attract insurers to pay for mindfulness and other psychological interventions that are known to work for pain.

Other young investigators who were runners up for the Max prize were Eric Bair, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, US, who described his work identifying patient subtypes in the OPPERA study, and Lisa Kilpatrick, University of California, Los Angeles, US, who described the first study looking at changes in resting-state brain connectivity in 82 women with interstitial cystitis/painful bladder syndrome compared to 85 healthy controls. Overall, her results so far suggest that women with this disease may have altered attention to visceral input and changes in sensorimotor functional connectivity to areas normally linked to pelvic floor function that are different from healthy women.

In addition to these talks there was a comprehensive overview of the TRPV1 channel structure from keynote speaker David Julius, University of California, San Francisco, US, and additional presentations from Linda Watkins, University of Colorado at Boulder, US, and Clifford Woolf, Boston Children’s Hospital, US. An archived webcast of the entire meeting is available on the NIH website (view Day 1 and Day 2). The complete meeting agenda is here.

http://www.painresearchforum.org/news/42863-pain-and-depression-comorbidity-conundrum

No comments:

Post a Comment

All comments welcome but advertising your own service or product will unfortunately result in your comment not being published.